Have you ever wondered why some people are naturally inclined to help others while others tend to be more selfish? A recent study has found that people attribute prosocial behaviors to genetics more strongly than antisocial ones. This finding has important implications for how we think about altruism and cooperation. It also suggests that we may be more likely to excuse antisocial behavior if we think it’s due to genetics. In this blog post, we will explore the implications of this study and what it means for our understanding of human nature.
The study conducted
The study conducted by the University of Chicago found that people are more likely to attribute prosocial behaviors, such as cooperation and helping others, to genetics than they are to attribute antisocial behaviors, such as aggression and theft. The study’s authors say that this may be because people want to believe that prosocial behavior is innate and unchangeable, while antisocial behavior is a choice that can be changed.

The results of the study
According to a recent study, people are more likely to attribute positive behaviors to genetics rather than negative behaviors. The study, conducted by a team of researchers at the University of Toronto, looked at how people perceive the role of genetics in both pro-social and anti-social behaviors.
The researchers found that people were more likely to attribute positive behaviors, such as altruism and cooperation, to genetics than they were negative behaviors, such as aggression and selfishness. This was especially true when the behavior in question was something that the person themselves had exhibited.
The study’s lead author, Professor Noam Shpancer, said that the findings could have important implications for how we think about genes and behavior. “If people tend to see pro-social behaviors as being under genetic control, then they may be more likely to support policies that focus on promoting these behaviors,” he said. ” Conversely, if people see anti-social behaviors as being under genetic control, they may be less likely to support policies that seek to change them.”
These findings suggest that our perceptions of the role of genetics in human behavior are not always accurate and can be influenced by our own biases. It is important to be aware of these biases when interpreting research on the topic.
The implications of the study
The study found that people were more likely to attribute prosocial behaviors, like altruism and cooperation, to genetics than antisocial ones, like aggression and crime. This has implications for how we think about and explain human behavior.
It suggests that we are more likely to see altruistic or cooperative behavior as innate and fixed, while we are more likely to see aggressive or criminal behavior as a choice or something that can be changed. This difference in how we view these behaviors can have real-world consequences.
For example, if we see altruistic behavior as something that is genetically determined, then we may be less likely to try to change it or encourage it in others. On the other hand, if we see aggressive or criminal behavior as something that is a choice, then we may be more likely to try to prevent it or intervene when it occurs.
This study highlights the importance of understanding how people perceive the causes of human behavior. Our perceptions can influence our actions and decisions, which can have real-world implications for individuals and society as a whole.

Why people attribute pro-social behavior to genetics more than anti-social
There are several reasons why people might attribute pro-social behavior to genetics more than anti-social behavior. First, pro-social behavior is generally seen as more desirable than anti-social behavior, so it makes sense that people would want to believe that pro-social behavior is genetic. Second, pro-social behavior is often seen as more natural or “innate” than anti-social behavior, which may lead people to believe that it is more likely to be determined by genes. Finally, research has shown that people tend to attribute dispositional traits (including both pro- and anti-social traits) to genetics more than situational factors. Therefore, it is not surprising that people would attribute pro-social Behavior to genetics more strongly than antisocial ones.
How this affects our understanding of human behavior
People tend to attribute positive behaviors to genetics and negative behaviors to environment. For example, if someone does something helpful, people are more likely to say it’s because they were born that way. If someone does something harmful, people are more likely to say it’s because of their upbringing or the company they keep.
This tendency has implications for how we understand human behavior. If we think positive behaviors are “hardwired” into our DNA, then we may be less likely to change them. On the other hand, if we think negative behaviors are due to environmental factors, then we may be more likely to try to change them.
Is prosocial behavior genetic?
Prosocial behaviors like generosity, kindness, and helping others in need are often seen as being hardwired into our DNA. And while there is some evidence that genetic factors can play a role in these behaviors, it’s important to keep in mind that they’re not the whole story.
For one thing, prosocial behaviors often run in families, which could be due to shared genes or simply the result of learning from parents and other relatives. Additionally, culture and environment can also influence whether or not someone displays prosocial behavior.
So while it’s true that genetics may be a small part of the picture when it comes to explaining why some people are more inclined to help others, it’s certainly not the whole story.
What is the difference between antisocial and prosocial behavior?
Most people can tell the difference between antisocial and prosocial behavior. Antisocial behavior is typically characterized by aggression, violence, and a general disregard for the wellbeing of others. On the other hand, prosocial behavior is typically characterized by cooperation, helpfulness, and compassion.
There is some debate over whether or not antisocial behavior is truly a separate construct from pro-social behavior. Some researchers believe that antisocial behavior is simply extreme pro-sociality – that is, people who engage in antisocial behaviors are actually trying to help themselves or others in some way, but they go about it in an inappropriate or harmful manner. However, most experts agree that there is a meaningful distinction between the two constructs.
The key difference between antisocial and prosocial behavior lies in intentionality. Prosocial behaviors are intentional – we perform them because we want to help another person or improve the world around us in some way. Antisocial behaviors, on the other hand, are usually not intentional – they’re often impulsive and motivated by our own self-interests rather than a desire to help others.
Of course, this isn’t always black and white – there are many gray areas when it comes to intentionality and motivation. But in general, if a behavior is motivated by self-interest ( even if it ultimately benefits someone else), it’s more likely to be classified as antisocial.

Can antisocial behavior be inherited?
There is a great deal of debate surrounding the idea of whether or not antisocial behavior can be inherited. Some people believe that it is entirely genetic, while others contend that environment plays a much larger role. The truth is likely somewhere in the middle.
It is important to remember that genes are not destiny. Just because someone has a genetic predisposition for antisocial behavior does not mean that they will automatically become a criminal or engage in other harmful activities. However, it does mean that they may be more likely to do so than someone without those genes.
There are many different factors that contribute to antisocial behavior, and it is often hard to pinpoint any one cause. It is likely that a combination of genetic and environmental factors play a role in shaping someone’s behavior.
What factors influence prosocial behavior?
When it comes to understanding why people help others, or don’t, a new study finds that people tend to emphasize the role of genes for altruistic behavior, but not for selfish behavior.
The study, published in the journal Nature Human Behavior, provides some of the first experimental evidence that people view genetic explanations differently for altruism versus selfishness.
“We found that when thinking about the genetics of altruistic behaviors – like helping a stranger or giving blood – people were much more likely to say that these behaviors are influenced by genes than when thinking about the genetics of selfish behaviors – like taking candy from a baby or harming someone you don’t know,” said co-author Christopher Oveis, an assistant professor of psychology at The University of Southern California Dornsife College of Letters, Arts and Sciences.
Oveis and his colleagues conducted six studies with more than 2,000 participants from around the world. In one experiment, they asked people to rate how much they believed various altruistic and selfish behaviors were influenced by genes. In another experiment, they asked people to explain why someone might engage in each type of behavior. And in a third experiment, they had people read fictional news stories about research on the genetics of altruism and selfishness.
Across all three experiments, the researchers found that people were more likely to say that genes influence altruistic behavior than selfish behavior.